Anyone who has set
out to learn a second language probably learned how to politely say, “I don’t
understand,” early on in their study.
Learning a second language is a process that comes with an abundant
supply of ambiguity and uncertainty. For
some learners this is what makes second language acquisition (SLA) so exciting,
while for others this same supply of ambiguity is what makes SLA
so frustrating. There are many learner
variables that have been studied in light of their effect on SLA. Learners vary on how tolerant they are of
ambiguity, and this variable seems to have a very real impact on learners’
success with SLA. The purpose of this paper is to review
literature that seeks to define tolerance of ambiguity, and to explore just how
ambiguity is encountered in SLA. From there, the purpose is to critically
examine research which correlates SLA success with varying degrees of ambiguity
tolerance, and review strategies and modifications which yield SLA success for students with varying degrees of
ambiguity tolerance.
Defining Tolerance of Ambiguity
Many studies have
been done on the tolerance of ambiguity in relation to SLA. Each attempts to define this
characteristic. According to Carol
Chapelle and Cheryl Roberts (1986) ambiguity tolerance (AT) is “a person’s
ability to function rationally and calmly in a situation in which
interpretation of all stimuli is not clear” (p.30). Thus a person with low AT is likely to view
uncertainty with psychological discomfort.
Ely (1989) defined AT as one’s acceptance of confusing situations and a
lack of clear lines of demarcation (Grace, 1998; Oxford, 1993). Thus a person with a high AT is likely to accept
uncertainty with ease. According to
Cynthia White (1999), AT “is a response formulated by the learner to feelings
of uncertainty or confusion, whereby the uncertainty is accommodated so that it
does not obstruct progress” (p.451).
Here it is emphasized that learners must respond with patience,
endurance, and confidence in order to display a high AT (White, 1999). Ehrman (1993) has offered perhaps the most
detailed definition of tolerance of ambiguity.
In her definition, she included a three fold model including: 1) the
ability to take in new information; 2) the ability to hold contradictory or
incomplete information; and 3) the ability to adapt one’s personal schemata as
a response to new material (Ehrman, 1993).
All of these definitions seem to indicate that a learner’s tolerance of
ambiguity involves the degree to which that learner is comfortable with
uncertainty or confusion and can respond with ease despite ambiguity.
Personality Tendencies and Tolerance of Ambiguity
Beyond the simple definition of
ambiguity tolerance (AT), it is important to note that it is often defined as a
personality variable. Research seems to
agree that certain measured personality characteristics are indications of how
tolerant a person may be when encountering uncertainty. El-Koumy (2000) conducted a study of Egyptian
college freshmen reading English and stated that AT is closely related to
anxiety and risk-taking, which are also personality characteristics. Other
studies of the AT variable have examined it in light of the Myers-Briggs Type
Inventory (MBTI), which is commonly used to measure personality types. Grace (1998) studied vocabulary retention in
light of AT and computer assisted language learning. Through this study, it was discovered that
when students learned vocabulary with computer supported translation options
(thus unambiguously), personality did not have a significant impact on the
amount of vocabulary retained. However,
when the translation option was not allowed and thus the vocabulary learning was
more ambiguous, personality was a significant factor in vocabulary
retention. In regards to the MBTI, she
found that sensing types were less tolerant of ambiguity than intuitive types;
judging types were less tolerant of ambiguity than perceiving types; and
thinking types were less tolerant of ambiguity than feeling types. These finding are also supported by Alastair
Sharp’s (2004) study of language learning and personality types in China. In addition to the variables studied by
Grace, Sharp used the MBTI to examine the introvert and extrovert variable and
found that extroverted students are likely to have a higher tolerance of
ambiguity due to their comfort level with risk-taking (Sharp, 2004). The
sensing and intuitive personality types’ relation to AT is also supported by
Felder and Henriques (1995). It would
seem that tolerance of ambiguity is directly related to a learner’s personality
type. This finding can be very helpful
to instructors who wish to learn about their students’ level of AT, in that a
personality inventory may be used.
No comments:
Post a Comment